Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

dc.creatorRíos H.S.
dc.creatorCeballos E.C.
dc.creatorGuerrero M.G.
dc.date.accessioned2020-03-26T16:32:54Z
dc.date.available2020-03-26T16:32:54Z
dc.date.issued2013
dc.identifier.citationActa Colombiana de Psicologia; Vol. 16, Núm. 1; pp. 101-113
dc.identifier.issn01239155
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12585/9083
dc.description.abstractThis article presents empirical evidence of variability and change in the performance of young children in causality tasks, highlighting the relevance of nonlinear perspective in studies about the first year of life. Using a longitudinal (12 weeks of observation) and cross-sectional (four age groups) design, the strategy trajectories used by110 children in solving the tasks adapted from the Operatory Causality Subscale of the Uzgiris- Hunt Functioning Scale (Uzgiris & Hunt, 1975) are described. For data treatment and analysis mixed methods were employed. From the qualitative perspective, the Situation Analysis Method was used to identify the cognitive demands that situations require. Subsequently, by means of a microgenetic analysis, the action programs of children were classified into three types of strategies: (1) Non Resolution, (2) Exploration, and (3) Resolution. Additionally, knowledge formats underlying these strategies were established. From a quantitative perspective, the strategy trajectories were analyzed by clusters (k-means).The clusters obtained with this analysis allowed to quantify the kind of strategies used by children in each resolution event of the Operative Causality Subscale. Results show: (a) the variability of strategies in the performance trajectories that make up each cluster, and (b) the change of strategies (e.g. from exploration to resolution) as an adaptive developmental resource. Finally, a case study is presented to illustrate the cognitive shift through the transition from implicit knowledge to explicit knowledge in a recurring phase model.eng
dc.format.mediumRecurso electrónico
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.language.isoeng
dc.publisherUniversidad Catolica de Colombia
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
dc.sourcehttps://www.scopus.com/inward/record.uri?eid=2-s2.0-84901669382&partnerID=40&md5=b38a5e414dea12b26cb1dde9f8eedf54
dc.titleChange and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life
dc.title.alternativeCambio y variabilidad: Un marco de referencia en los estudios sobre el primer año de vida
dcterms.bibliographicCitationBaillargeon, R., De Vos, J., Object permanence in 3,5 and 4,5 months old infants: Further evidence (1991) Child Development, 62, pp. 1227-1246
dcterms.bibliographicCitationBates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, T., Camaioni, L., Volterra, V., (1979) The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy, , N.Y.: Academic Press
dcterms.bibliographicCitationBower, T., (1984) El mundo Perceptivo del Niño, , Madrid: Morata
dcterms.bibliographicCitationBruner, J., From communication to language: A psychological perspective (1975) Cognition, 3, pp. 255-328
dcterms.bibliographicCitationCohen, L.B., Oakes, L.M., How infants perceive a simple causal event (1993) Developmental Psychology, 29, pp. 421-433
dcterms.bibliographicCitationCourage, M., Howe, M., From infant to child. The dynamics of cognitive change in the second year of life (2002) Psychological Bulletin, 128 (2), pp. 250-277
dcterms.bibliographicCitationFischer, K., Bidell, T., Developmental of psychological structures in action and thought (1998) Handbook of Child Psychology. Vol. 1: Theoretical Models of Human Development, pp. 467-561. , En W. Damon and R.M. Lerner (Eds), N.Y.: John Wiley and Sons
dcterms.bibliographicCitationFlynn, E., Siegler, R., Measuring Change: Current trends and future directions in microgenetic research (2007) Infant and Child Development, 16, pp. 135-149
dcterms.bibliographicCitationFodor, J., (1983) La Modularidad de la mente, , Barcelona: Gedisa
dcterms.bibliographicCitationKarmiloff-Smith, A., (1994) Más allá de la Modularidad. La Ciencia Cognitiva desde la Perspectiva del Desarrollo., , Madrid: Alianza Editorial
dcterms.bibliographicCitationMandler, J., How to build a baby II: Conceptual primitives (1992) Psychological Review, 99, pp. 587-604
dcterms.bibliographicCitationMandler, J., Representation (1998) Handbook of Child Psychology. Cognition, Perception, and Language, 2, pp. 255-308. , Fifth edition, En D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), N.Y.: Wiley
dcterms.bibliographicCitationMeltzoff, A.N., Moore, M.K., Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures (1983) Child Development, 54, pp. 702-709
dcterms.bibliographicCitationMiller, P.H., Order in variability, variability in order: Why it matters for theories of development (2002) Human Development, 45, pp. 161-166
dcterms.bibliographicCitationMiller, P.H., Coyle, T.R., Developmental change: Lessons from microgenesis (1999) Conceptual Development: Piaget's Legacy, pp. 209-239. , En E.K. Scholnick, K. Nelson, S.A. Gelman & P.H. Miller (Eds.), Mahwah, N. J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
dcterms.bibliographicCitationOrozco, M., Ochoa, S., Sanchez, H., (2001) Prácticas Culturales y Educación de la Niñez, , Bogotá: Fundación Antonio Restrepo Barco
dcterms.bibliographicCitationOrozco, M., Perinat, A., Sánchez, H., Cognitive Development and Interaction Context (2009) Living in Poverty: Developmental Poetics of Cultural Realities, pp. 287-308. , En A. C. Bastos & E. Prabinovich (Eds.), Charlott, NC: Information Age Publishing
dcterms.bibliographicCitationPascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J., The psychological unit and its role in task analysis: A reinterpretation of object permanence (1991) Criteria for Competence: Controversies in the Conceptualization and Assessment of Children's Abilities, pp. 151-187. , En M. Chandler, & M. Chapman (Eds.), Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Earlbaun Associates
dcterms.bibliographicCitationPerner, J., (1994) Comprender la Mente Representacional., , Barcelona: Paidos
dcterms.bibliographicCitationPiaget, J., (1985) La Toma de Conciencia, , Madrid: Morata
dcterms.bibliographicCitationPiaget, J., (1989) La Construcción de lo Real en el Niño, , Barcelona
dcterms.bibliographicCitationCrítica, S.A., Piaget, J., (1990) El Nacimiento de la Inteligencia en el niño, , Barcelona: Editorial Crítica
dcterms.bibliographicCitationPuche-Navarro, R., (2000) Formación de herramientas científicas en el niño pequeño, , Santafé de Bogotá: Arango Editores
dcterms.bibliographicCitationPuche-Navarro, R., (2003) El niño que Piensa y Vuelve a Pensar, , Cali: Artes Gráficas del Valle Editores
dcterms.bibliographicCitationPuche-Navarro, R., Érase una vez el desarrollo (2008) Claves para pensar el cambio: Ensayo sobre psicología del desarrollo, pp. 27-69. , En F. Larreamendy-Joerns, R. Puche & A. Restrepo (Eds.), Bogotá: Uniandes-CESO
dcterms.bibliographicCitationSánchez, H., Guevara, M., Cerchiaro, E., Desarrollo y/o cambio de la noción de objeto permanente y causalidad operatoria: Evidencia empírica en el primer año de vida, , (en prensa), Aceptado para publicación en la revista Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana
dcterms.bibliographicCitationSiegler, R.S., Crowley, K., The microgenetic method: A direct means for studying cognitive development (1991) American Psychologist, 46, pp. 606-620
dcterms.bibliographicCitationSiegler, R.S., Cognitive variability: A key to understanding cognitive development (1994) Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3 (1), pp. 1-5
dcterms.bibliographicCitationSiegler, R., (1996) Emerging minds: The process of change in children's thinking, , N.Y.: Oxford University Press
dcterms.bibliographicCitationSiegler, R., Variability and infant development (2002) Infant Behavior & Development, 25, pp. 550-557
dcterms.bibliographicCitationSiegler, R., Microgenetic analyses of learning (2006) Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language, pp. 464-510. , En W. Damon, R. M. Lerner, D. Kuhn, and R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Hoboken, NJ: Wiley
dcterms.bibliographicCitationUzgiris, Y., Hunt, J., (1975) Assessment in infancy: Ordinal Scales of psychological development., , Chicago: University of Illinois Press
dcterms.bibliographicCitationYan, Z., Fischer, K., Always under construction: Dynamic variations in adult cognitive microdevelopment (2002) Human Development, 45, pp. 141-160
datacite.rightshttp://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_16ec
oaire.resourceTypehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501
oaire.versionhttp://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85
dc.type.driverinfo:eu-repo/semantics/article
dc.type.hasversioninfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
dc.subject.keywordsCognitive change
dc.subject.keywordsCognitive development
dc.subject.keywordsOperative causality
dc.subject.keywordsUzgiris Hunt Scale
dc.subject.keywordsVariability
dc.rights.accessrightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/restrictedAccess
dc.rights.ccAtribución-NoComercial 4.0 Internacional
dc.identifier.instnameUniversidad Tecnológica de Bolívar
dc.identifier.reponameRepositorio UTB
dc.type.spaArtículo
dc.identifier.orcid56185436000
dc.identifier.orcid24461215800
dc.identifier.orcid56185476200


Ficheros en el ítem

FicherosTamañoFormatoVer

No hay ficheros asociados a este ítem.

Este ítem aparece en la(s) siguiente(s) colección(ones)

Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/

Universidad Tecnológica de Bolívar - 2017 Institución de Educación Superior sujeta a inspección y vigilancia por el Ministerio de Educación Nacional. Resolución No 961 del 26 de octubre de 1970 a través de la cual la Gobernación de Bolívar otorga la Personería Jurídica a la Universidad Tecnológica de Bolívar.